Skip to contents

This package provides dataframes of information about the consonants and vowels in American English. The following datasets collect acquisition (acq) features which (try to) characterize the expected acquisition or speech-motor difficulty of speech sounds. See also data_features_consonants.

Usage

data_acq_consonants

Format

An object of class tbl_df (inherits from tbl, data.frame) with 24 rows and 10 columns.

Details

Crowe and McLeod (2020) norms for English consonant acquisition

Crowe and McLeod (2020, below as the cm2020_ variables) provides a systematic review and summary statistics for age of acquisition norms for English consonants. They scoured the literature of acquisition ages for individual consonants and computed summary statistics on them. They considered just accuracy of sounds when produced in single words. Their sources include a mix of a journal articles and norms for articulation assessments. They do not weight statistics from individual studies by sample size or sampling procedure.

I prepared the Crowe and McLeod (2020) data by copying the relevant numbers from their Table 2 making the following changes: 1) rounding mean and SD values to 1 decimal point (3 days for ages in months), 2) dropping /ʍ/, 3) using /r/, /g/, /tʃ/, /dʒ/ for IPA characters instead of the specialized characters used in the article.

The early 8, middle 8 and late 8 (Shriberg, 1993)

The English consonants are often broken down into three developmental classes, based on Shriberg (1993):

  • Early 8: m b j n w d p h

  • Middle 8: t ŋ k g f v tʃ dʒ,

  • Late 8: ʃ θ s z ð l r ʒ

This classification is included as the s93_eights column.

From these names alone, we might interpret these classes such that sounds in the Early 8 would be acquired before the ones in the Middle 8, and likewise that the Middle 8 would be acquired before the Late 8. But these classes were not created by examining patterns of typical consonant acquisition.

For some context, Shriberg (1993) introduces the Early 8, Middle 8, and Late 8 data by describing the following panel of the article's Figure 7:

About which, Shriberg (1993) says: "The values for this trend, which is a profile of consonant mastery, were taken from a group of 64 3- to 6-year-old speech-delayed children Shriberg, Kwiatkowski, & Gruber, 1992). Severity of involvement of the 24 English consonants is represented as the percentage correct for each consonant sorted in decreasing order from left to right. Notice that the most obvious breaks in this function allow for a division of the 24 consonants into three groups of eight sounds termed the Early-8, averaging over 75% correct, the Middle-8, averaging 25%-75% correct, and the Late-8, including consonants averaging less than 25% correct in continuous conversational speech (/ʒ/ is infrequently represented in young, speech-delayed children's spontaneous conversational speech)."

So, there were 64 3–6-year-old children with speech delays, and consonant sounds were divided into three classes based on how often these children produced the sounds correctly on average in a conversational speech sample. This classification is not so much a measure of the relative ordering of speech sound development as it is the relative difficulty of these sounds for children with a speech delay of unknown origin. It would be more appropriate to replace the levels of Early/Middle/Late with Easy/Medium/Hard.

Consonant acquisition features

data_acq_consonants provides the following features:

knitr::kable(data_acq_consonants)

phonecmubetwiscbetcm2020_90_age_meancm2020_90_age_sdcm2020_90_age_mincm2020_90_age_maxcm2020_90_num_studiescm2020_90_stages93_eights
pPp33.26.9244812earlyearly
bBb31.47.8244813earlyearly
tTt38.59.2246013earlymiddle
dDd35.76.7244813earlyearly
kKk37.77.3244813earlymiddle
gGg36.86.6244813earlymiddle
CHtsh53.510.7367212middlemiddle
JHdzh51.011.8367213middlemiddle
mMm33.26.7244813earlyearly
nNn33.17.4244813earlyearly
ŋNGng40.310.8245510earlymiddle
fFf38.36.3244813earlymiddle
vVv50.810.8366612middlemiddle
θTHth77.07.4729610latelate
ðDHdh69.011.3549612latelate
sSs51.316.3248412middlelate
zZz56.814.3308411middlelate
ʃSHsh55.010.5367212middlelate
ʒZHzh70.712.260843latelate
hHHh35.07.0244813earlyearly
lLl53.810.4246012middlelate
rRr66.618.6309612latelate
wWw35.26.8244813earlyearly
jYj45.811.0306013earlyearly

Description of each column:

phone

phone in IPA

cmubet

phone in the CMU alphabet

wiscbet

phone in an older system used by our lab

cm2020_90_age_mean, cm2020_90_age_sd, cm2020_90_age_min, cm2020_90_age_max

Age of acquisition statistics reported by Crowe & McLeod (2020). Statistics are the mean, SD, min and max age (in months) when children reached 90% accuracy on a consonant.

cm2020_90_num_studies

Number of studies used by Crowe & McLeod (2020) to compute the corresponding statistics.

cm2020_90_stage

Developmental stage assigned to the consonant by Crowe & McLeod (2020). Sounds with an age_mean before 48 months are early, before 60 months are middle, and of 60 or older are late.

s93_eights

Developmental stage of Shriberg (1993)—that is, the early 8, middle 8 and late 8 consonants.

References

Crowe, K., & McLeod, S. (2020). Children’s English Consonant Acquisition in the United States: A Review. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 29(4), 2155–2169. https://doi.org/10.1044/2020_AJSLP-19-00168

Shriberg, L. D. (1993). Four New Speech and Prosody-Voice Measures for Genetics Research and Other Studies in Developmental Phonological Disorders. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 36(1), 105–140. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3601.105