References

Allopenna, P. D., Magnuson, J. S., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1998). Tracking the time course of spoken word recognition using eye movements: Evidence for continuous mapping models. Journal of Memory and Language, 38(4), 419–439. doi:10.1006/jmla.1997.2558

Altvater-Mackensen, N., & Mani, N. (2013). The impact of mispronunciations on toddler word recognition: Evidence for cascaded activation of semantically related words from mispronunciations of familiar words. Infancy, 18(6), 1030–1052. doi:10.1111/infa.12022

Arias-Trejo, N., & Plunkett, K. (2009). Lexical-semantic priming effects during infancy. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 364, 3633–3647. doi:10.1098/rstb.2009.0146

Baayen, R. H., Rij, J. van, Cat, C. de, & Wood, S. N. (2016). Autocorrelated errors in experimental data in the language sciences: Some solutions offered by generalized additive mixed models. Retrieved from http://arxiv.org/abs/1601.02043

Bailey, T. M., & Plunkett, K. (2002). Phonological specificity in early words. Cognitive Development, 17(2), 1265–1282. doi:10.1016/S0885-2014(02)00116-8

Barr, D. J. (2008). Analyzing ‘visual world’ eyetracking data using multilevel logistic regression. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 457–474. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2007.09.002

Barton, D. (1976). Phonemic discrimination and the knowledge of words in children under three years. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development, 11, 61–68.

Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 1–48. doi:10.18637/jss.v067.i01

Baylis, A. L., Munson, B., & Moller, K. T. (2008). Factors affecting articulation skills in children with velocardiofacial syndrome and children with cleft palate or velopharyngeal dysfunction: A preliminary report. The Cleft Palate-Craniofacial Journal, 45(2), 193–207. doi:10.1597/06-012.1

Bion, R. A. H., Borovsky, A., & Fernald, A. (2013). Fast mapping, slow learning: Disambiguation of novel word–object mappings in relation to vocabulary learning at 18, 24, and 30 months. Cognition, 126(1), 39–53. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2012.08.008

Blomquist, C., & McMurray, B. (2017). War of the words: Development of inter-lexical inhibition in typical children. The University of Iowa. Retrieved from https://ir.uiowa.edu/honors_theses/31

Borovsky, A., Elman, J. L., & Fernald, A. (2012). Knowing a lot for one’s age: Vocabulary skill and not age is associated with anticipatory incremental sentence interpretation in children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 112(4), 417–436. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2012.01.005

Bürkner, P.-C. (2017). brms: An R package for Bayesian multilevel models using Stan. Journal of Statistical Software, 80(1), 1–28. doi:10.18637/jss.v080.i01

Charles-Luce, J., & Luce, P. A. (1990). Similarity neighbourhoods of words in young children’s lexicons. Journal of Child Language, 17(1), 205–215. doi:10.1017/S0305000900013180

Charles-Luce, J., & Luce, P. A. (1995). An examination of similarity neighbourhoods in young children’s receptive vocabularies. Journal of Child Language, 22(3), 727–735. doi:10.1017/S0305000900010023

Chow, J., Aimola Davies, A. M., & Plunkett, K. (2017). Spoken-word recognition in 2-year-olds: The tug of war between phonological and semantic activation. Journal of Memory and Language, 93, 104–134. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2016.08.004

Coady, J. A., & Aslin, R. N. (2003). Phonological neighbourhoods in the developing lexicon. Journal of Child Language, 30(2), 441–469. doi:10.1017/S0305000903005579

Cristia, A., Seidl, A., Junge, C., Soderstrom, M., & Hagoort, P. (2014). Predicting individual variation in language from infant speech perception measures. Child Development, 85(4), 1330–1345. doi:10.1111/cdev.12193

Dollaghan, C. A. (1994). Children’s phonological neighbourhoods: Half empty or half full? Journal of Child Language, 21(2), 257–271. doi:10.1017/S0305000900009260

Ellis Weismer, S., Haebig, E., Edwards, J. R., Saffran, J. R., & Venker, C. E. (2016). Lexical processing in toddlers with ASD: Does weak central coherence play a role? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(12), 3755–3769. doi:10.1007/s10803-016-2926-y

Fennell, C. T., & Waxman, S. R. (2010). What paradox? Referential cues allow for infant use of phonetic detail in word learning. Child Development, 81(5), 1376–83. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01479.x

Fernald, A., & Marchman, V. A. (2012). Individual differences in lexical processing at 18 months predict vocabulary growth in typically developing and late-talking toddlers. Child Development, 83(1), 203–222. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01692.x

Fernald, A., Swingley, D., & Pinto, J. P. (2001). When half a word is enough: Infants can recognize spoken words using partial phonetic information. Child Development, 72(4), 1003–15. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00331

Gabry, J., & Goodrich, B. (2018). RStanARM: Bayesian applied regression modeling via Stan. Retrieved from https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=rstanarm

Gabry, J., & Mahr, T. (2018). bayesplot: Plotting for Bayesian models. Retrieved from https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=bayesplot

Gamer, M., Lemon, J., & Singh, I. F. P. (2012). irr: Various coefficients of interrater reliability and agreement. Retrieved from https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=irr

Gelman, A., & Carlin, J. (2014). Beyond power calculations: Assessing Type S (sign) and Type M (magnitude) errors. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9(6), 641–651. doi:10.1177/1745691614551642

Gelman, A., & Hill, J. (2007). Data analysis using regression and multilevel/hierarchical models. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Gelman, A., Carlin, J. B., Stern, H. S., Dunson, D. B., Vehtari, A., & Rubin, D. B. (2014). Bayesian data analysis (3rd ed.). Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press/Taylor & Francis Group.

Horst, J. S., Samuelson, L. K., Kucker, S. C., & McMurray, B. (2011). What’s new? Children prefer novelty in referent selection. Cognition, 118(2), 234–244. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2010.10.015

Huang, Y. T., & Snedeker, J. (2011). Cascading activation across levels of representation in children’s lexical processing. Journal of Child Language, 38(03), 644–661. doi:10.1017/S0305000910000206

Huettig, F., Rommers, J., & Meyer, A. S. (2011). Using the visual world paradigm to study language processing: A review and critical evaluation. Acta Psychologica, 137(2), 151–171. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.11.003

Kapnoula, E. C., Packard, S., Gupta, P., & McMurray, B. (2015). Immediate lexical integration of novel word forms. Cognition, 134, 85–99. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2014.09.007

Kruschke, J. K., & Liddell, T. M. (2017). The Bayesian New Statistics: Hypothesis testing, estimation, meta-analysis, and power analysis from a Bayesian perspective. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1–29. doi:10.3758/s13423-016-1221-4

Lany, J. (2017). Lexical-processing efficiency leverages novel word learning in infants and toddlers. Developmental Science. doi:10.1111/desc.12569

Law, F., II, & Edwards, J. R. (2015). Effects of vocabulary size on online lexical processing by preschoolers. Language Learning and Development, 11(4), 331–355. doi:10.1080/15475441.2014.961066

Law, F., II, Mahr, T., Schneeberg, A., & Edwards, J. R. (2016). Vocabulary size and auditory word recognition in preschool children. Applied Psycholinguistics. doi:10.1017/S0142716416000126

Leach, L., & Samuel, A. G. (2007). Lexical configuration and lexical engagement: When adults learn new words. Cognitive Psychology, 55(4), 306–353. doi:10.1016/j.cogpsych.2007.01.001

Lew-Williams, C., & Fernald, A. (2007). Young children learning Spanish make rapid use of grammatical gender in spoken word recognition. Psychological Science, 18(3), 193–8. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01871.x

Magnuson, J. S., Mirman, D., & Myers, E. (2013). Spoken word recognition. In D. Reisberg (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of cognitive psychology. Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195376746.013.0027

Magnuson, J. S., Tanenhaus, M. K., Aslin, R. N., & Dahan, D. (2003). The time course of spoken word learning and recognition: Studies with artificial lexicons. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132(2), 202–227. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.132.2.202

Mahr, T., & Edwards, J. R. (2018). Using language input and lexical processing to predict vocabulary size. Developmental Science, e12685. doi:10.1111/desc.12685

Mahr, T., McMillan, B. T. M., Saffran, J. R., Ellis Weismer, S., & Edwards, J. (2015). Anticipatory coarticulation facilitates word recognition in toddlers. Cognition, 142, 345–350. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2015.05.009

Mani, N., & Plunkett, K. (2010). In the infant’s mind’s ear: Evidence for implicit naming in 18-month-olds. Psychological Science, 21(7), 908–913. doi:10.1177/0956797610373371

Mani, N., Durrant, S., & Floccia, C. (2012). Activation of phonological and semantic codes in toddlers. Journal of Memory and Language, 66(4), 612–622. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2012.03.003

Marchman, V. A., & Fernald, A. (2008). Speed of word recognition and vocabulary knowledge in infancy predict cognitive and language outcomes in later childhood. Developmental Science, 11(3), F9–16. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00671.x

Markman, E. M., & Wachtel, G. F. (1988). Children’s use of mutual exclusivity to constrain the meanings of words. Cognitive Psychology, 20(2), 121–157. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(88)90017-5

Marslen-Wilson, W. D., & Zwitserlood, P. (1989). Accessing spoken words: The importance of word onsets. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15(3), 576–585. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.15.3.576

Mather, E., & Plunkett, K. (2012). The role of novelty in early word learning. Cognitive Science, 36(7), 1157–1177. doi:10.1111/j.1551-6709.2012.01239.x

Mayor, J., & Plunkett, K. (2014). Infant word recognition: Insights from TRACE simulations. Journal of Memory and Language, 71(1), 89–123. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2013.09.009

McClelland, J. L., & Elman, J. L. (1986). The TRACE model of speech perception. Cognitive Psychology, 18(1), 1–86. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(86)90015-0

McElreath, R. (2016). Statistical rethinking: A Bayesian course with examples in R and Stan. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press/Taylor & Francis Group.

McMurray, B., Horst, J. S., & Samuelson, L. K. (2012). Word learning emerges from the interaction of online referent selection and slow associative learning. Psychological Review, 119(4), 831–877. doi:10.1037/a0029872

McMurray, B., Samelson, V. M., Lee, S. H., & Tomblin, J. B. (2010). Individual differences in online spoken word recognition: Implications for SLI. Cognitive Psychology, 60(1), 1–39. doi:10.1016/j.cogpsych.2009.06.003

Mervis, C. B., & Bertrand, J. (1994). Acquisition of the novel name–nameless category (N3C) principle. Child Development, 65(6), 1646–1662. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1994.tb00840.x

Mirman, D. (2014). Growth curve analysis and visualization using R. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press/Taylor & Francis Group.

Mirman, D., Dixon, J. A., & Magnuson, J. S. (2008). Statistical and computational models of the visual world paradigm: Growth curves and individual differences. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 475–494. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2007.11.006

Mirman, D., Yee, E., Blumstein, S. E., & Magnuson, J. S. (2011). Theories of spoken word recognition deficits in Aphasia: Evidence from eye-tracking and computational modeling. Brain and Language, 117(2), 53–68. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2011.01.004

Oleson, J. J., Cavanaugh, J. E., McMurray, B., & Brown, G. (2017). Detecting time-specific differences between temporal nonlinear curves: Analyzing data from the visual world paradigm. Statistical Methods in Medical Research, 26(6), 2708–2725. doi:10.1177/0962280215607411

R Core Team. (2018). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Retrieved from https://www.R-project.org/

Rigler, H., Farris-Trimble, A., Greiner, L., Walker, J., Tomblin, J. B., & McMurray, B. (2015). The slow developmental time course of real-time spoken word recognition. Developmental Psychology, 51(12), 1690–1703. doi:10.1037/dev0000044

Seedorff, M., Oleson, J. J., & McMurray, B. (2018). Detecting when timeseries differ: Using the Bootstrapped Differences of Timeseries (BDOTS) to analyze Visual World Paradigm data (and more). Journal of Memory and Language, 102, 55–67. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2018.05.004

Sóskuthy, M. (2017). Generalised additive mixed models for dynamic analysis in linguistics: a practical introduction. Retrieved from http://arxiv.org/abs/1703.05339

Stager, C. L., & Werker, J. F. (1997). Infants listen for more phonetic detail in speech perception than in word-learning tasks. Nature, 388(6640), 381. doi:10.1038/41102

Stan Development Team. (2017). Stan modeling language: User’s guide and reference manual.

Swingley, D. (2009). Onsets and codas in 1.5-year-olds’ word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 60(2), 252–269. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2008.11.003

Swingley, D., & Aslin, R. N. (2000). Spoken word recognition and lexical representation in very young children. Cognition, 76(2), 147–66. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00081-0

Swingley, D., & Aslin, R. N. (2002). Lexical neighborhoods and the word-form representations of 14-month-olds. Psychological Science, 13(5), 480–484. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00485

Swingley, D., & Aslin, R. N. (2007). Lexical competition in young children’s word learning. Cognitive Psychology, 54(2), 99–132. doi:10.1016/j.cogpsych.2006.05.001

Swingley, D., Pinto, J. P., & Fernald, A. (1999). Continuous processing in word recognition at 24 months. Cognition, 71(2), 73–108. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00021-9

van Rij, J., Wieling, M., Baayen, R. H., & van Rijn, H. (2017). itsadug: Interpreting time series and autocorrelated data using GAMMs.

Weisleder, A., & Fernald, A. (2013). Talking to children matters: Early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary. Psychological Science, 24(11), 2143–52. doi:10.1177/0956797613488145

Werker, J. F., Cohen, L. B., Lloyd, V. L., Casasola, M., & Stager, C. L. (1998). Acquisition of word–object associations by 14-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 34(6), 1289. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.34.6.1289

White, K. S., & Morgan, J. L. (2008). Sub-segmental detail in early lexical representations. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(1), 114–132. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2008.03.001

Winter, B., & Wieling, M. (2016). How to analyze linguistic change using mixed models, growth curve analysis and generalized additive modeling. Journal of Language Evolution, 1(1), 7–18. doi:10.1093/jole/lzv003

Wood, S. N. (2017). Generalized additive models: An introduction with R (2nd ed.). Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press/Taylor & Francis Group.